Tack Box Talk

Sane and Sound: The story of a equine geneticists goal to build a better horse

June 10, 2024 Kris Hiney, Samantha Brooks Season 6 Episode 135
Sane and Sound: The story of a equine geneticists goal to build a better horse
Tack Box Talk
More Info
Tack Box Talk
Sane and Sound: The story of a equine geneticists goal to build a better horse
Jun 10, 2024 Season 6 Episode 135
Kris Hiney, Samantha Brooks

Dr. Samantha Brooks, world renown equine geneticist from the University of Florida joins us for a free ranging talk on how horses get their markers, how we might someday be able to breed for temperament and how even your temperament may predict how you experience pain.  While we never really pick a topic, this is a great discussion on how gene's influence your horse's behavior.

Show Notes Transcript

Dr. Samantha Brooks, world renown equine geneticist from the University of Florida joins us for a free ranging talk on how horses get their markers, how we might someday be able to breed for temperament and how even your temperament may predict how you experience pain.  While we never really pick a topic, this is a great discussion on how gene's influence your horse's behavior.

Kris Hiney: So I think I wanted just to do it broadly and try to relate, maybe like, where we at with horses compared to everybody getting their dogs, DNA, to see if, like his cousin lives down the road or like

 

35

00:05:42.303 --> 00:05:56.199

Kris Hiney: yes, or where are we? Do people need to pay money, and I don't wanna get off the track of yours. But I now like now they have, like you could get their microbiome tested, and you could do this like? Are we anywhere where all of this is

 

36

00:05:56.910 --> 00:05:58.170

Kris Hiney: helpful

 

37

00:05:58.760 --> 00:06:00.630

Kris Hiney: for the average horse owner.

 

38

00:06:03.050 --> 00:06:04.850

Samantha Brooks: For the average horse center. Okay.

 

39

00:06:05.150 --> 00:06:06.699

Kris Hiney: Is that kinda.

 

40

00:06:07.680 --> 00:06:12.070

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah. Let me think, just 1 s, because I

 

41

00:06:12.970 --> 00:06:14.850

Samantha Brooks: just gave this talk

 

42

00:06:15.582 --> 00:06:17.630

Samantha Brooks: a couple weeks ago.

 

43

00:06:17.770 --> 00:06:33.208

Samantha Brooks: And now I'm trying to remember. What did I talk about? But the whole topic was, I don't remember my own talk. But, this happens to me all the time. It's really fun. My like, my students bring me stuff. And they're like, look at this paper. And I'm like, Oh, that's really good! Who wrote it?

 

44

00:06:34.390 --> 00:06:34.929

Samantha Brooks: And then.

 

45

00:06:34.930 --> 00:06:36.385

Kris Hiney: And then it's you.

 

46

00:06:37.880 --> 00:06:42.041

Samantha Brooks: So I get to. It's fun. I get to rediscover my science all the time, cause I forget it. Now.

 

57

00:07:48.290 --> 00:07:51.270

Samantha Brooks: Alright. So the 3 topics that I picked

 

58

00:07:51.900 --> 00:07:58.099

Samantha Brooks: in stock horses was Pssm. One. And I just this morning took a look at the raw data. So we just did a

 

59

00:07:58.500 --> 00:08:07.799

Samantha Brooks: genetics topics survey of the general public. And that that's what I'm presenting on Sunday. So I was like, Oh, I guess I should probably do some, at least some summary statistics.

 

60

00:08:08.577 --> 00:08:24.472

Samantha Brooks: And we've got about 500 respondents, and about 200 of those were stock horse people, and I was really surprised to see that they are very concerned with PSSm. Type one and hYPP, and I don't know. I guess I thought hip was old news now, but

 

00:09:22.510 --> 00:09:24.176

Samantha Brooks: No, not 2,009, 2,000, and

 

76

00:09:26.500 --> 00:09:39.470

Samantha Brooks: it was 25 years after the discovery of the Allele. But in those 25 years it went from an allele frequency or an affected frequency of something like 20% to 59% in the halter horses.

 

77

00:09:39.939 --> 00:09:45.709

Samantha Brooks: So yes, they definitely use the tool in the wrong way. I guess I'm shocked that the

 

78

00:09:46.240 --> 00:09:48.550

Samantha Brooks: selective advantage is so strong.

 

79

00:09:48.550 --> 00:09:53.181

Kris Hiney: It is. They're freaks like, I, I guess. Have you been in a halter barn?

 

80

00:09:53.490 --> 00:09:56.879

Samantha Brooks: Oh, yes, I like sides of beef, sides of beef, and.

 

81

00:09:56.880 --> 00:09:57.480

Kris Hiney: Yeah.

 

82

00:09:57.480 --> 00:10:09.823

Samantha Brooks: We have this argument in our program all the time about marketability of stock horses. And you know, we everybody's got Ocd, we're no, we're no difference. And I'm like, Well, this is a solvable problem. Stop feeding them.

 

83

00:10:10.705 --> 00:10:32.670

Samantha Brooks: And and they're like, Yeah, if we do, if they're too small, they won't bring a good enough price when we go to auction them off. And I'm like, Well, there's you know what they look like, and if they're gonna anyways. And if they're gonna function. So yeah, it's freakish. So I guess that's it is. It's is that it's the the moth to the flame effect. But I'm surprised. So our question was worded

 

84

00:10:32.910 --> 00:10:40.730

Samantha Brooks: of these genetic conditions, which one is which oh, which it's like, which one is most concerning.

 

85

00:10:41.090 --> 00:10:41.450

Kris Hiney: And.

 

86

00:10:41.450 --> 00:10:49.119

Samantha Brooks: And I thought concerning would mean things that they felt were dangerous in some way. But I guess that's not. Guess they don't feel it's dangerous, because

 

87

00:10:50.802 --> 00:10:54.259

Samantha Brooks: anyways, science is easy. People are hard. Yeah.

 

88

00:10:54.260 --> 00:10:57.352

Kris Hiney: Yeah. The ethics of what one does with the science is,

 

89

00:10:57.610 --> 00:11:11.484

Samantha Brooks: Right? Yeah, yeah, this is the thing. We made a nice test. But okay, so so, stock horse wise, we can certainly talk about hypp if you want, but pssm, one is the one that I chose.

 

90

00:11:12.570 --> 00:11:21.210

Samantha Brooks: for a couple of reasons, and then for sport, horses, race horses truthfully, but also sport horses to some degree.

 

91

00:11:21.802 --> 00:11:24.879

Samantha Brooks: There's a genetic region for height

 

92

00:11:24.930 --> 00:11:30.509

Samantha Brooks: that is also important for development of OCD. And development of roaring, both of which are

 

93

00:11:30.620 --> 00:11:39.119

Samantha Brooks: pretty expensive and career ending even for a trail horse bad. Ocd. Is going to turn into hock arthritis, which is going to turn into injections or else walk

 

94

00:11:39.180 --> 00:11:40.699

Samantha Brooks: trot only, and

 

95

00:11:41.070 --> 00:11:45.820

Samantha Brooks: those kind of things. And then for sport, horse type

 

96

00:11:46.850 --> 00:11:59.210

Samantha Brooks: people I was talking about fragile, foal syndrome, which is now a big advantage. Well, there's several papers showing. It's it's advantageous for dressage. And so it's going to be the same situation as HYPP.

 

97

00:11:59.240 --> 00:12:01.670

Samantha Brooks: where 2 copies you're pretty much dead.

 

98

00:12:01.740 --> 00:12:11.299

Samantha Brooks: One copy everybody wants to breed for, and the Allele frequency is gonna start to sky Skyrocket right? And 

 

99

00:12:12.630 --> 00:12:17.239

Samantha Brooks: the Pssm. And that height locus

 

100

00:12:17.860 --> 00:12:25.830

Samantha Brooks: are both kind of nice, because they have dietary management strategies to help to mitigate the negatives. And so, even if you have a

 

101

00:12:25.920 --> 00:12:27.999

Samantha Brooks: a fairly mature horse.

 

102

00:12:28.650 --> 00:12:37.000

Samantha Brooks: You know they carry. PSSM, you can make some fairly simple management changes to try to avoid

 

103

00:12:37.310 --> 00:12:45.879

Samantha Brooks: ending up with an episode of of PSSM. Which can be painful and expensive, and in some cases life-threatening. So

 

104

00:12:46.290 --> 00:12:49.139

Samantha Brooks: that was my tack previously was that

 

105

00:12:50.390 --> 00:12:52.859

Samantha Brooks: even if you have an adult horse and you're not a breeder.

 

106

00:12:53.450 --> 00:12:58.749

Samantha Brooks: You can learn some things about your horse to change the things that you to change the way you handle it day to day.

 

107

00:12:59.090 --> 00:13:00.260

Samantha Brooks: That will

 

108

00:13:00.810 --> 00:13:04.955

Samantha Brooks: improve your horse's performance and wellbeing, and hopefully also your

 

109

00:13:05.600 --> 00:13:06.579

Samantha Brooks: wallet.

 

110

00:13:08.110 --> 00:13:10.360

Samantha Brooks: your economic sustainability.

 

111

00:13:10.360 --> 00:13:11.530

Kris Hiney: Gotcha gotcha.

 

112

00:13:11.530 --> 00:13:23.130

Samantha Brooks: So to speak. But there's a lot of topics we could go to on this. But yeah, why should the everyday? And then, of course there are ancestry products for the horse that are just as good, if not better, than what's in the dog. It's just.

 

113

00:13:23.560 --> 00:13:25.780

Samantha Brooks: I don't think people know, to look for it.

 

114

00:13:27.560 --> 00:13:30.180

Kris Hiney: and is it helpful? Right? Is it.

 

115

00:13:31.170 --> 00:13:34.299

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah, yeah. And is it helpful?

 

116

00:13:34.880 --> 00:13:41.689

Kris Hiney: Cause, I think, yeah, I mean, the difference is, so many people have mixed free dogs, which I don't think there's a plethora of people getting mixed horses just.

 

117

00:13:41.690 --> 00:13:48.118

Samantha Brooks: Well, what is a breed, though? Here's  the whole. Now we get into philosophy. What is a breed? Because

 

118

00:13:48.440 --> 00:13:50.331

Kris Hiney: Association tells you it is.

 

119

00:13:51.070 --> 00:13:58.039

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, it's a piece of paper. Yeah, breed is as heritable as the paper. That darn pedigree is printed on.

 

120

00:13:58.992 --> 00:14:03.359

Samantha Brooks: It really is so so you can have something that's registered. You don't really know what it is.

 

121

00:14:03.500 --> 00:14:28.799

Samantha Brooks: I have a great example for that. My daughter's horse came to us with no papers. After going through 5 owners at some point we heard a rumor. She went through an auction in Texas. She went through several sets of hands, and we got her, and she's a cute little bay thing with a star, and looks vaguely stock horse to me. But she has these lovely, beautiful kind of hazel colored eyes. And so, me being a geneticist, I'm like, Oh, this is curious.

 

122

00:14:29.130 --> 00:14:41.789

Samantha Brooks: So I knew. I know there's a locus for he, for light colored eyes. They call it a tiger eye, and I thought, Well, we should test for that. So while I was doing that, I said, well, we're just going to do. We know nothing about her. So we did a full ancestry panel. We probably should just.

 

123

00:14:41.860 --> 00:14:50.569

Samantha Brooks: I don't know. You can edit this right. We did a full ancestry panel on her. And I did comprehensive genetic testing, because I didn't know enough about her

 

124

00:14:50.750 --> 00:14:52.720

Samantha Brooks: breed, so to speak.

 

125

00:14:52.800 --> 00:14:54.359

Samantha Brooks: to decide

 

126

00:14:54.730 --> 00:14:55.710

Samantha Brooks: what

 

127

00:14:56.810 --> 00:14:59.470

Samantha Brooks: targeted panel would be appropriate. And

 

128

00:14:59.640 --> 00:15:04.779

Samantha Brooks: these days genetic testing is cheaper if you buy in bulk. So I was like, just do the whole thing.

 

129

00:15:05.810 --> 00:15:08.980

Samantha Brooks: Well, it turns out she is

 

130

00:15:09.260 --> 00:15:17.050

Samantha Brooks: probably a stock horse based on her ancestry. She genetically, is half thoroughbred, full 50%. So

 

131

00:15:17.290 --> 00:15:21.190

Samantha Brooks: typical for a like a performance type, quarter horse

 

132

00:15:21.310 --> 00:15:24.709

Samantha Brooks: for a pleasure type, quarter horse or stock horse

 

133

00:15:24.830 --> 00:15:39.300

Samantha Brooks: type group, including the paint horses. And when we looked at her genetic testing for known for known traits. She didn't have anything for light colored eyes. What she did have was a frame overo Allele.

 

134

00:15:39.770 --> 00:15:40.620

Kris Hiney: And.

 

135

00:15:40.970 --> 00:15:42.900

Samantha Brooks: She's Bay with

 

136

00:15:42.990 --> 00:15:45.490

Samantha Brooks: a small star and a small sock.

 

137

00:15:46.440 --> 00:15:50.850

Samantha Brooks: No, no bright colored markings really whatsoever.

 

138

00:15:51.560 --> 00:16:01.039

Samantha Brooks: for us. That's valuable information to know my my daughter begs me to breathe the mayor every year. I don't know that I will, because I'm not made of money, but

 

139

00:16:01.568 --> 00:16:08.750

Samantha Brooks: she is such a spectacular 4 h horse, just a trooper, and knock on wood sound as dollar.

 

140

00:16:08.820 --> 00:16:30.829

Samantha Brooks: So you know a as a genetic system like she's got some merit but if we were to have chosen, say, a nice paint horse stallion, and weren't careful, we could have ended up with in a in a very lethal white syndrome full, who would die shortly after birth, and it would have no clue to that. That was a possibility, because she does not carry the overt phenotype

 

141

00:16:30.940 --> 00:16:34.139

Samantha Brooks: of of an of of a paint horse. So

 

142

00:16:34.330 --> 00:16:40.979

Samantha Brooks: my! Based on those clues. I suspect she was bred in a paint horse program, and.

 

143

00:16:40.980 --> 00:16:41.450

Kris Hiney: And be.

 

144

00:16:41.450 --> 00:16:41.880

Samantha Brooks: Because.

 

145

00:16:41.880 --> 00:16:42.350

Kris Hiney: She did.

 

146

00:16:42.600 --> 00:16:50.660

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, she did not have qualifying white enough now. They'll accept genetic testing, but she's in her 15 or so years old

 

147

00:16:51.000 --> 00:16:59.639

Samantha Brooks: back. Then she couldn't have gotten registered, based solely on a genetic test. So she probably got discarded as a solid paint.

 

148

00:17:00.310 --> 00:17:07.690

Samantha Brooks: and that fills us in a little bit about, you know. Just so. We know a little bit more about this horse that we spend so much time with, and

 

149

00:17:08.510 --> 00:17:13.069

Samantha Brooks: fortunately she does not have an HYPP. Or a Pssm. Or

 

150

00:17:13.329 --> 00:17:28.723

Samantha Brooks: something health-wise, that that could be potentially alarming. But that that frame of arrow is is definitely a. It's a curiosity, and and if I win the lottery tomorrow and decide to go into horse breeding and decide to breed her, I definitely know a little bit better about how I should.

 

151

00:17:29.450 --> 00:17:30.800

Samantha Brooks: I should target that.

 

152

00:17:30.800 --> 00:17:38.270

Kris Hiney: So as the geneticist. Why, what controls, whether that overo gene is expressed or not expressed.

 

153

00:17:38.703 --> 00:17:40.870

Samantha Brooks: Well, ask mother nature, cause.

 

154

00:17:40.870 --> 00:17:41.350

Kris Hiney: It is not.

 

155

00:17:41.350 --> 00:17:41.940

Samantha Brooks: Telling.

 

156

00:17:41.940 --> 00:17:45.449

Kris Hiney: No, I'm asking Dr. Samantha Brooks, equine, genetic expert.

 

157

00:17:45.800 --> 00:17:48.110

Samantha Brooks: Oh, the expert, the expert!

 

158

00:17:48.710 --> 00:18:13.859

Samantha Brooks: Alright! Well, you know. So during development, your Milano cytes migrate in that tiny little embroyo they migrate from a population of stem cells that evolves along what we call the neural crest which lies where the spinal column eventually will form, and in some ways. It makes sense, right? Cause. Neurons are these cells that have lots of branches

 

159

00:18:13.860 --> 00:18:27.849

Samantha Brooks: and melanocytes or cells that have lots of branches. They're little factories that make pigment, and then they ship that pigment out along the branches, out to the the follicles that are gonna make hair and the skin cells that that need pigment.

 

160

00:18:28.320 --> 00:18:39.935

Samantha Brooks: So they start just along the top line of the embryo, and then they respond to migratory signals. These chemical roadmaps that are

 

161

00:18:40.500 --> 00:18:59.840

Samantha Brooks: secreted by the so the destination cells and the melanocytes. Actually, I've got a great video of melanocytes labeled with fluorescent dye on a little mouse embryo. But they literally flow out and around the body of the embryo to get to their final location.

 

162

00:19:00.162 --> 00:19:25.970

Samantha Brooks: So many things can disrupt those chemical signals in lots of horses, for both genetic and random reasons. They don't make it all the way to the ends of those limbs, and we end up with socks right? And in an embryo the nose is actually pretty far to get to, too. So you end up with a blaze. Those markings are common in those locations, because in the developing embryo they're actually quite far from from where the melanocytes

 

163

00:19:26.270 --> 00:19:27.690

Samantha Brooks: start their journey.

 

164

00:19:28.070 --> 00:19:40.300

Samantha Brooks: So if you, if you disrupt those chemical signals in a number of different ways. You end up with these different sort of flow patterns where the migration has been disrupted at certain periods of time.

 

165

00:19:40.822 --> 00:20:07.740

Samantha Brooks: Things like Tobiano get these sort of big puddles in one place or another, and frame of arrow is kind of odd, because it seems like they kind of start, and then they kind of get distracted and and don't seem to finish out and and don't both flow, and then fill in across the the skin surface. So to keep with that analogy, if you think about like, if you were to take a bucket of paint and throw it out across the concrete floor.

 

166

00:20:08.390 --> 00:20:19.519

Samantha Brooks: Even if you had 10 opportunities to do this. This sounds like a very therapeutic, asmr kind of activity, right? But if you had 10 opportunities to throw that bucket of paint

 

167

00:20:19.640 --> 00:20:31.220

Samantha Brooks: every time you do it. It's it's not going to be the same right? So so each time you have a foal, and these pigment cells begin to migrate. It's going to be a slightly unique pattern in that animal.

 

168

00:20:31.760 --> 00:20:43.830

Samantha Brooks: So as it turns out in frame overo. If we set a human defined threshold, like like the traditional limits for white markings in the American Paint Horse.

 

169

00:20:44.230 --> 00:21:07.759

Samantha Brooks: that just by random chance, about 9 out of 10 horses that carry one frame of arrow. Allele will make it beyond that threshold, and we'll have some fairly dramatic and exciting markings on them. But one out of every 10 times. So 10% of frame of overo horses do not have markings that reach that threshold, and many of them have

 

170

00:21:08.060 --> 00:21:25.669

Samantha Brooks: very plain markings like our little mare. And it it's just, you know, it's just part of that, that mystery that is a Mother nature and that random game of chance that sometimes they make it, sometimes they don't. Now

 

171

00:21:26.020 --> 00:21:32.830

Samantha Brooks: other patterns like Tobiano. You can think about it. Tobiano just has a bigger market of paint to start with, right? So

 

172

00:21:33.100 --> 00:21:43.880

Samantha Brooks: something around 99% of Tobiano horses will make it beyond that threshold. But there are a very small proportion of even Tobiano horses who carry one allele

 

173

00:21:44.120 --> 00:22:06.220

Samantha Brooks: that don't have markings that would be overtly recognizable as characteristic of of Tobiano. We call those Crypto Tobiano, sometimes because they are sort of surprising when they happen. But it's all part of that normal bell curve distribution. Right? So among those overos, 10% will be really spectacular

 

174

00:22:06.500 --> 00:22:16.639

Samantha Brooks: white markings that are very dramatic, and 10% will be fairly plainly marked, and most of them fall in sort of that average bin

 

175

00:22:16.970 --> 00:22:18.230

Samantha Brooks: of markings.

 

176

00:22:18.230 --> 00:22:35.999

Kris Hiney: So would we am I correct? Is that epigenetics, or is it not? So like, maybe we'll try to explain. There's your genetics that like that that doesn't change right? And then there's like all these other things that happen that affect how your genes are expressed. So is that

 

177

00:22:36.600 --> 00:22:37.280

Kris Hiney: part of that.

 

178

00:22:37.280 --> 00:22:37.780

Samantha Brooks: Right.

 

179

00:22:37.780 --> 00:22:39.469

Kris Hiney: And I misrepresent.

 

180

00:22:39.470 --> 00:22:43.319

Samantha Brooks: No, no, it it can be part of. It's just random chance.

 

181

00:22:43.600 --> 00:22:56.970

Samantha Brooks: Part of it is that basket. Of all the other things that can can alter genetics. So those can include influences from other genes. So gene by gene interactions, influences from the environment

 

182

00:22:57.628 --> 00:23:18.459

Samantha Brooks: and then epigenetics itself are regulatory changes that are carried on the DNA that are added in response to certain environmental triggers, but particularly during very critical periods, right around fertilization and an early embryonic development. So that's epigenetics, or these

 

183

00:23:18.490 --> 00:23:24.729

Samantha Brooks: changes in the way the DNA is packaged to try to prepare that embryo for a life that might be somewhat challenging.

 

184

00:23:25.200 --> 00:23:36.589

Samantha Brooks: So there are gene by gene interactions for things even as simple as coat color and white spotting the easiest one to talk about is chestnut versus, not chestnut.

 

185

00:23:36.890 --> 00:23:41.430

Samantha Brooks: So if you look at patterns like Tobiano and fame overo

 

186

00:23:41.720 --> 00:23:48.910

Samantha Brooks: both of them, the horses will statistically, significantly, have more white markings. If they are on a

 

187

00:23:48.970 --> 00:24:05.470

Samantha Brooks: a chestnut base coat color than if they're on a bay or black base coat color. And the biological reasoning for that is that chestnut itself results from a genetic alteration that disrupts the function of a switch called MC one r.

 

188

00:24:05.620 --> 00:24:17.639

Samantha Brooks: and that switch turns on the ability to make black pigment, and when it's defective it's stuck in the off position. So our chestnut horses have a slightly defective black pigment on switch.

 

189

00:24:17.740 --> 00:24:37.062

Samantha Brooks: That switch is also important for cell signaling. And so in mice and rats and humans in horses. If you take those pigment cells that have a defective switch and put them into culture. They're kind of lazy pigment cells. The the chestnut pigment cells are just not as prolific for

 

190

00:24:38.661 --> 00:25:01.529

Samantha Brooks: increasing their population and growing in a dish, and all those those kind of activities. So it makes sense that that after they make their migration, those chestnut pigment cells are a little bit lazy, so they don't. They don't multiply as fast. They don't cover the horses completely. And so chestnut plus mini white spotting patterns equals more white spottings than your average horse.

 

191

00:25:01.960 --> 00:25:10.869

Samantha Brooks: Okay? And then there, there could be environmental things shown gene by environment. It's not well documented in verses, but it is well documented in mice

 

192

00:25:10.960 --> 00:25:19.330

Samantha Brooks: that another nutritional impact is methionine, which is important in the pathway for making pigment.

 

193

00:25:19.510 --> 00:25:31.899

Samantha Brooks: So in mice, if you supplement methionine and certain genetic backgrounds to the the mama mouse. The baby mice that are growing in utero will experience

 

194

00:25:31.900 --> 00:25:56.590

Samantha Brooks: changes in their coat color that will last throughout the rest of the life of the little baby mouse. They're they're really cute, because if if they on one side of the dietary spectrum, they get these puffy little orange chestnut mice we caught we call little pumpkin mice, cause they were so cute, and and then, with the right amount of supplementation, you get dark coated, kind of like, almost like bay colored mice that are very thin and lean so

 

195

00:25:56.650 --> 00:26:10.619

Samantha Brooks: that nutritional impact, probably through epigenetics and the environmental impact acting through epigenetics, changes the programming for genes that are important both for pigmentation and for metabolism.

 

196

00:26:11.469 --> 00:26:21.589

Samantha Brooks: There's many, many different mechanisms that will alter gene regulation, most of which we've never had the opportunity to properly study in the in the horse.

 

197

00:26:23.350 --> 00:26:27.019

Samantha Brooks: so there! It's complicated. The the expert says it's complicated.

 

198

00:26:27.364 --> 00:26:50.119

Kris Hiney: That's fine, like, I think that that actually is, is super important for people that are breeding their horses because they need to know that right? So that example of that solid Bay Mare like you need to know, or why. Sometimes you get this surprise like what the heck? Where did these colors come from right. They were hiding on that bell curve. I guess.

 

199

00:26:50.120 --> 00:27:15.238

Samantha Brooks: Right? Yeah, absolutely. They can hide on the bell curve for sure. And then you can have gene by gene interactions that will hide some of these phenotypes that can come out. You know I get people who aren't that excited about genetics will tell me. Well, I don't want to do it, because I think horse breeding is an art, and I feel like if I you know, if I do, the genetics is gonna take all the fun out of it. And and first of all, I congratulate them on being independently wealthy, because

 

200

00:27:15.865 --> 00:27:42.490

Samantha Brooks: many of us breed horses as a business, and so we need to be a little bit. If we're going to survive as a business, we have no choice but to be more strategic about it. But then, second of all I say, you know, even with the best planning, even if you very carefully pick those genotypes and sire and dam, it's still a flip of the coin as to which alleles that baby gets so there will always be surprises, despite all of our best laid plans. So

 

201

00:27:42.920 --> 00:27:50.229

Samantha Brooks: genetics will never take the fun or challenge completely out of horse breeding. It just lets us be a little more strategic about it.

 

202

00:27:50.230 --> 00:27:57.180

Kris Hiney: Right. And I think you know what in the horse industry, maybe what's frustrating for people. One is, we're probably not looking for that.

 

203

00:27:57.180 --> 00:28:20.999

Kris Hiney: The traits that are as heritable as in other like milk production. And, rate of gain, we just don't have these measurable concrete things right? That like, if you're a winner or a loser. There's a lot that goes into it versus a measurement that a whole bunch of researchers can take on a population over time. Isn't that. What kind of murk murkeys us up a bit.

 

204

00:28:21.950 --> 00:28:28.729

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, you know, that is a that is a huge challenge for us right now. And it's not that I think it's

 

205

00:28:28.950 --> 00:28:36.309

Samantha Brooks: it's not doable as much as we simply don't have the the tools or the industry-wide agreement. I mean.

 

206

00:28:37.350 --> 00:28:47.079

Samantha Brooks: most dairy men will agree. A cow needs to make milk, you know. Then you can have discussions about somatic cell count, or about milk. Fat? Yes, but most of them agree. They need to make milk

 

207

00:28:47.110 --> 00:29:10.059

Samantha Brooks: when it comes to horses. It's hard to get 3 horse people in a room and get them all to agree about what's the most valuable thing in horses. But typically those are hard things to measure things like performance. They are, they are difficult, they are multifactorial. They have huge influences due to environment, but we know it is in part genetic.

 

208

00:29:10.448 --> 00:29:33.399

Samantha Brooks: A draft horse is not going to necessarily win the triple crown, but by the same token, Thoroughbred horse is not gonna survive long pulling a plow. So we know that we as as humans have shaped the genetics of these breeds to alter performance, we can do it. So there are genes.

 

209

00:29:33.680 --> 00:29:40.641

Samantha Brooks: There definitely are genes involved. Right? So part of what I've done in my research program lately is is is

 

210

00:29:41.540 --> 00:29:50.580

Samantha Brooks: as we've had to sort of work a little bit more strategically because of the severe lack of research funding right now.

 

211

00:29:50.900 --> 00:29:57.870

Samantha Brooks: I've decided you know what I'm I'm not. I'm not so fascinated with working on genetic diseases, because there's always another one of those.

 

212

00:29:58.560 --> 00:30:12.390

Samantha Brooks: There's always another one of those to study. So at the moment we are focused on developing new ways to measure the 2 traits that I decided were most important to horse owners. And I got to decide, because, you know.

 

213

00:30:12.390 --> 00:30:15.006

Kris Hiney: Yours right? I choose.

 

214

00:30:15.530 --> 00:30:24.900

Samantha Brooks: That's right. I choose. But I think I chose pretty good because I decided, broadly speaking, we're focusing. The 2 traits are sane and sound.

 

215

00:30:24.960 --> 00:30:47.350

Samantha Brooks: And it's just that it's just that simple, right? So we developed a behavioral assay to look at startle response. So that initial subconscious reaction that can trigger fear related behaviors and horses, and anyone who's a horse person has been subject to the fear plus gravity effect which ends up with

 

216

00:30:47.400 --> 00:30:58.779

Samantha Brooks: rider on the ground so, or rider getting knocked down, or a driven horse taking off at the cart, leaving Driver behind.

 

217

00:30:59.070 --> 00:31:12.339

Samantha Brooks: So most people agree that that that's a pretty important trait to focus on. So what is a sound, or what is a sane mind? And how does it work? And and we focus on the reflexive part, because that's the easiest corner to work

 

218

00:31:12.760 --> 00:31:22.019

Samantha Brooks: and then soundness. So we were starting to use some AI tools to try to track locomotion, because when we were asking about locomotor traits as they related to performance.

 

219

00:31:22.600 --> 00:31:29.550

Samantha Brooks: There were just simply no tools available to us, even even in in clinical equine, medicine.

 

220

00:31:29.750 --> 00:31:31.260

Samantha Brooks: and orthopedics.

 

221

00:31:31.540 --> 00:31:34.659

Samantha Brooks: The best tool we have right now for detecting

 

222

00:31:34.720 --> 00:31:37.080

Samantha Brooks: changes in limb function is still

 

223

00:31:37.110 --> 00:31:46.600

Samantha Brooks: the human eye, which is not very sensitive, not very repeatable, and subject to all sorts of of bias. So

 

224

00:31:47.880 --> 00:31:48.530

Samantha Brooks: yeah.

 

225

00:31:48.530 --> 00:31:59.166

Kris Hiney: I think there's a new tool coming, though that's better than maybe the lameness locator that is hopefully in the works. You're tied with that.

 

226

00:31:59.560 --> 00:32:04.149

Samantha Brooks: No? Well, there's a commercial. Yeah. There's a commercial activity in Europe.

 

227

00:32:04.700 --> 00:32:16.459

Samantha Brooks: That has started to work on a cell phone app, which I I think, as terms of an idea is a great idea. They I'm not sure their approach is necessarily the right one. We're going a different angle on that.

 

228

00:32:16.850 --> 00:32:24.020

Samantha Brooks: We are using all free type software applications so freely available software applications

 

229

00:32:24.080 --> 00:32:32.300

Samantha Brooks: and reinventing the wheel, so to speak. So rather than relying on the characteristics that the human eye

 

230

00:32:32.360 --> 00:32:48.820

Samantha Brooks: typically uses. We are going straight for the quantitative measures, things like joint range of motion. And how much time that foot spends on the ground. It's it's a harder road, to hoe to just discard the adaptive

 

231

00:32:49.410 --> 00:33:14.250

Samantha Brooks: characteristics that we have used as humans. But the the point of using something like a camera or a cell phone is that it is recordable. So 100 reevaluatable can be repeatable in the right set of hands, and it works at 120 frames per second. Human eye cannot see that fast. So we're we're gonna try to reinvent the wheel on that, and see see where it goes.

 

232

00:33:14.250 --> 00:33:18.335

Kris Hiney: So I have a question on the on the the behavior side. So

 

233

00:33:18.710 --> 00:33:31.410

Kris Hiney: anybody that listens to the podcast knows that I'm also a dog nut. So there have been. I'm sure you're familiar with all the dog genetic stuff, too, because it gets around. So the big controversy one is like this

 

234

00:33:31.700 --> 00:33:56.839

Kris Hiney: is behavior genetic. And so like the breed predictability. Because I I I hear it all the time right? So that the breed does not predict behavior at all, every individual is an individual. And I'm gonna I'm gonna let you kind of. I'm gonna give my little 2 cents and then ask you how you think about it. But to me

 

235

00:33:57.130 --> 00:34:06.690

Kris Hiney: it is predictive. Right? So the breed what you've genetically selected for is predictive of a lot of things. So

 

236

00:34:07.010 --> 00:34:15.650

Kris Hiney: where we at with horses is that a factor, or are we still? Every individual is an individual.

 

237

00:34:16.130 --> 00:34:26.110

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, well, I'll tell you. This debate is not unique to horses and dogs. I was just reviewing some literature and human psychology yesterday for some grant writing that I'm doing.

 

238

00:34:26.219 --> 00:34:35.619

Samantha Brooks: And you know, they are just coming around to this this concept that there is more

 

239

00:34:36.000 --> 00:34:47.289

Samantha Brooks: to the relationship between genetics and what we call temperament. So so, your temperament being sort of your baseline level of sensitivity and reactivity

 

240

00:34:47.440 --> 00:35:02.140

Samantha Brooks: than we ever previously appreciated, because in many of these studies they would often ignore things like environmental and developmental factors. Right? But if you conduct a thorough enough study to look at things like the environment.

 

241

00:35:02.190 --> 00:35:19.150

Samantha Brooks: All of a sudden you start to realize, hey, wait a minute. Genetics was actually underpinning a lot of this in in human psychology, where it becomes very interesting is we looked at 2 studies, one looking at at cardiovascular disease, now 3, actually one at cardiovascular disease, one at Alzheimer's disease, and one at Osteoarthritis

 

242

00:35:20.230 --> 00:35:27.300

Samantha Brooks: and for the 2 disease States. Having a underlying temperament that was more

 

243

00:35:27.630 --> 00:35:53.680

Samantha Brooks: sensitive and prone to anxiety, was also a very significant risk factor for cardiovascular disease and Alzheimer's disease, and when it came to osteoarthritis, the level of of limb function, and of that patient's perceived pain was quite a bit higher in underlying temperament styles that were more sensitive and prone to prone to anxiety.

 

244

00:35:54.010 --> 00:36:03.100

Samantha Brooks: So so when it comes to horses and dogs, we know there are a couple, a couple of of well known underlying genetic alleles that do

 

245

00:36:03.230 --> 00:36:09.569

Samantha Brooks: influence specific tendencies, but across breeds the theme tends to be that

 

246

00:36:09.800 --> 00:36:12.579

Samantha Brooks: genetics can set up

 

247

00:36:12.720 --> 00:36:15.400

Samantha Brooks: a a heritable temperament.

 

248

00:36:15.440 --> 00:36:17.579

Samantha Brooks: an underlying sort of

 

249

00:36:17.920 --> 00:36:20.540

Samantha Brooks: foundation that

 

250

00:36:20.680 --> 00:36:23.159

Samantha Brooks: will sort of dictate

 

251

00:36:23.700 --> 00:36:30.530

Samantha Brooks: under controlled circumstances what your reflexive actions are likely to be.

 

252

00:36:30.600 --> 00:36:34.700

Samantha Brooks: and then you can layer on top of that.

 

253

00:36:35.246 --> 00:36:57.720

Samantha Brooks: Early life, developmental and learning experiences so particularly like our in year experiences even and then. And then there's a lot that can be can be learned during infancy. And then on top of that, the influence of environment. So are you in a very stressful situation versus are you in a very supportive

 

254

00:36:58.180 --> 00:37:00.270

Samantha Brooks: type of environment.

 

255

00:37:00.340 --> 00:37:19.750

Samantha Brooks: and that they got those sort of 3 layers of the cake as what's really shaping what we perceive as animal owners and as fellow humans as that day to day behavior. And I think because of the 2 layers on top of it, genetics gets a little bit overlooked these days, and we thought, Oh, it's all has to do with.

 

256

00:37:19.770 --> 00:37:28.939

Samantha Brooks: You know what you've learned and and the situation you're in, and that's not necessarily true. Your your default programming is largely genetic, and it gets altered based on those 2 terms.

 

257

00:37:28.940 --> 00:37:37.407

Kris Hiney: So it'd be like the genetics is actually the cake. And then all those decorations they put on top and put the fondant on.

 

258

00:37:37.760 --> 00:38:04.865

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, right? Right? Yeah. So you walk up and you take a slice of white cake, you think, oh, this will be great. It'll be vanilla, and then you cut it open. You're like, Oh, never mind, is chocolate, you know. Yeah, that's that could be a good. It could be a good analogy. So specifically, in some preliminary data we presented at a meeting a couple of years ago, my Phd student, who is still we conduct this behavioral experiment every single year with our Uf. Full crop. So we've done it for 10 years. I'll probably have to do it for another 10 years before we get

 

259

00:38:05.110 --> 00:38:22.399

Samantha Brooks: full a full sample size we can analyze. But when she looked at the heritability, so the proportion of a given trait due to genetics just for that initial spook response. So we take our baby horses and we take an automatic umbrella and we pop it open at them.

 

260

00:38:22.460 --> 00:38:28.579

Samantha Brooks: and fun ensues. Right? But when we look at the first 3 s, so

 

261

00:38:28.900 --> 00:38:33.080

Samantha Brooks: did they did they react strongly to the umbrella or not

 

262

00:38:33.120 --> 00:38:35.969

Samantha Brooks: in her numbers. 2 thirds of that was genetics.

 

263

00:38:37.110 --> 00:38:38.250

Samantha Brooks: Pretty pretty high.

 

264

00:38:38.590 --> 00:38:48.319

Samantha Brooks: The interesting thing is is that when we look at the later behaviors like, did they spin and run? Did they stand nearby, chew a couple times and go back to eating.

 

265

00:38:48.652 --> 00:39:09.930

Samantha Brooks: Those later behaviors. The heritability drops off very, very quickly. So so it sets up this really elegant solution for me, for the average horse owner. And that, let's say you're going to buy a new horse, or you're you know you're looking for a horse for your daughter, or you're trying to decide what to do with a problematic horse. Should you send them to the trainer?

 

266

00:39:10.210 --> 00:39:22.550

Samantha Brooks: If we can identify those reactivity genes early on. So is, are they going to react? Are they not? Gonna well, if they're not gonna react, you probably don't want to turn them into a racehorse or barrel racer, or a cutting horse, because

 

267

00:39:22.720 --> 00:39:50.449

Samantha Brooks: they may not have the quick reflexes you want, but they could be a great 4 H horse right? And then in among those who are reactive type, then we know that environment and learned experience are going to be key. So I might a target that horse potentially for the professional who wants super quick reflexes. And B, I'm going to be especially cautious on how I train that animal, and because I know that those later gross motor behaviors had fairly low heritability.

 

268

00:39:50.820 --> 00:39:56.829

Samantha Brooks: I also know that I have a really great chance to be able to to

 

269

00:39:56.950 --> 00:40:00.190

Samantha Brooks: influence that horse's behavior through

 

270

00:40:00.220 --> 00:40:02.750

Samantha Brooks: targeted strategic training approaches.

 

271

00:40:02.750 --> 00:40:18.239

Kris Hiney: So, and and I don't wanna maybe extrapolate too much. But this is this is fun for me, cause I do really love the behavior side. So a. And I've always said, you know, there's certain training practices that in some horses like, essentially.

 

272

00:40:18.250 --> 00:40:24.119

Kris Hiney: you know, when they're desensitizing them, and and the ones you do it the wrong way. Right? So to me, those ones that have

 

273

00:40:24.140 --> 00:40:27.109

Kris Hiney: the look that higher, startle response

 

274

00:40:27.210 --> 00:40:31.790

Kris Hiney: tend to be the ones that retain fear memories

 

275

00:40:32.090 --> 00:40:51.270

Kris Hiney: more so, and are not good candidates for some, and that's where sometimes they sort a little bit along some genetic lines that I've been a believer of no, it. This is not a one fit. One. Size fits all because there's something unique about those horses, about how they

 

276

00:40:51.580 --> 00:40:59.969

Kris Hiney: then retain learning. Am I making too much out of this from that little bit? If there's a genetic piece to that startle response.

 

277

00:41:00.130 --> 00:41:13.530

Samantha Brooks: Alright. Well, I I am not a neuroscientist. But I do collaborate with them a lot, because I I really want to find a way to study these traits more closely. But yes, memories are are laid down

 

278

00:41:13.980 --> 00:41:26.369

Samantha Brooks: very effectively when they are associated with a strong emotional response and emotional responses typically coming from the amygdala in the brain. Right? So if you've got this horse, who has that reactive temperament type

 

279

00:41:26.470 --> 00:41:49.400

Samantha Brooks: and they get set off by, you know the wind blow. I'll tell you, I went on. A trail ride the other day, and my neighbor had put a bunch of feed sacks in the trash can to be picked up, and one of them had gotten loose, and my rock solid eventer, who never spooks anything. One of those feedbags caught the wind, and flipped up like right in front of us, like one of those clowns in the funhouse

 

280

00:41:49.430 --> 00:41:50.719

Samantha Brooks: at the fair.

 

281

00:41:50.750 --> 00:42:04.350

Samantha Brooks: And bless his heart, you know, he, we both had a I don't think it scared me more, and it scared him. But you have an unexpected reaction right? And if that in some horses is accompanied by that negative emotional

 

282

00:42:05.161 --> 00:42:16.579

Samantha Brooks: outpouring, so to speak. It's gonna be more likely to be held in memory because because of the tie to emotion into the activation and the amyg and the amygdala.

 

283

00:42:17.380 --> 00:42:30.299

Samantha Brooks: That means, yeah. You do have to be more careful with them, right? Because if they're going to be more likely to be triggered into an emotional response. You know you're going to be laying down some longer term

 

284

00:42:30.640 --> 00:42:53.959

Samantha Brooks: sort of effects than maybe with another horse and on the flip side. If they associate you personally with positive experiences and a positive emotional state, then they might form a more indelible bond with you as a handler, and might retain some of those skills that they learn while you're they're in a positive training environment

 

285

00:42:54.440 --> 00:42:59.989

Samantha Brooks: much longer than a horse who found the whole thing to be very blase. Right?

 

286

00:43:00.240 --> 00:43:03.010

Samantha Brooks: It's with great power comes great responsibility.

 

287

00:43:03.010 --> 00:43:21.899

Kris Hiney: Yeah, no, that's I just think this is super cool, because you're saying the science supports. What a a lot of hopefully good horsemen have been like, know those sensitive ones. You need to treat differently. But like there's legit reasons why that we need to do that and appreciate

 

288

00:43:21.900 --> 00:43:34.032

Kris Hiney: literally the genetic background of that animal. And when you just said, like those more amygdala driven like I own them. An emo, an amygdala amygdala! There we go. I know what the word.

 

289

00:43:34.370 --> 00:43:36.319

Samantha Brooks: That brain part back there. Yeah.

 

290

00:43:36.320 --> 00:43:49.330

Kris Hiney: Yeah, that little nut back there. I have an amygdala driven dog right? And so they are very different. Doesn't mean they're not trainable. But, man, you just gotta do things a little bit more

 

291

00:43:49.410 --> 00:43:50.720

Kris Hiney: carefully.

 

292

00:43:52.030 --> 00:44:21.579

Samantha Brooks: Right? Right? You do. You do. And you do for the behavioral reasons. And I think, as we have recently shown in humans, and I think, will show in horses. You need to do it for health reasons as well. Right like you know the behavior part, so can you. Can your horse do his job day to day, and then can you afford to keep him without having to have the ved out every other day and write a whole bunch of checks. Yeah, so it makes sense to really to pay attention to these things. They they're tough questions, like, when I'm writing a Grant

 

293

00:44:22.090 --> 00:44:28.639

Samantha Brooks: Grant agencies are tend to be more interested in something they could say, put in a syringe and use tomorrow.

 

294

00:44:28.660 --> 00:44:29.700

Kris Hiney: Hmm sure.

 

295

00:44:29.700 --> 00:44:37.080

Samantha Brooks: Shortsighted the industry as a whole. They're like, yes. What am I gonna do about my my horse? You know my fools that have rotococcus this season?

 

296

00:44:37.080 --> 00:44:37.529

Kris Hiney: Yeah.

 

297

00:44:37.980 --> 00:44:45.070

Samantha Brooks: And I'm like, I know. Oh, I know I get it. I totally get it. But I think that some of these bigger questions

 

298

00:44:45.180 --> 00:44:51.520

Samantha Brooks: have the potential for for greater payoff. It's just going to be more of a long term investment.

 

299

00:44:51.520 --> 00:45:03.879

Kris Hiney: Yeah, I'm just like that. It's more broadly applicable to so many different species. And and humans like, if you even know, like, your genetic background is, you're already a little on the twitchy side, like here's the things we can do.

 

300

00:45:03.880 --> 00:45:25.440

Samantha Brooks: Right? Right? Yeah, I saw some great work looking at humans and and their temperament style as it interact, and their temperament style being largely genetic in nature, and then their stress environment based on their academic. I'm sorry. Academic, that's not right. Their economic

 

301

00:45:26.280 --> 00:45:39.610

Samantha Brooks: been so were they within poverty or above the the Federal poverty threshold, and and the the more vulnerable personalities really excelled when they were above

 

302

00:45:40.005 --> 00:46:07.280

Samantha Brooks: poverty threshold, but were disproportionately, negatively impacted when they were below poverty, so that that sensitivity it was. It was both a super power and a a source of kryptonite, so to speak, given a specific environment, and as animal managers, we sometimes, as much as possible have the power to alter that environment. And if if we have a roadmap to help us decide how to alter it.

 

303

00:46:07.670 --> 00:46:14.890

Samantha Brooks: We can really make some measurable changes in the day-to-day quality of life for our horses and ourselves.

 

304

00:46:14.890 --> 00:46:29.459

Kris Hiney: Well, it's even. And it's nice, because it sounds like this the genetic background, which is really, I think, helpful to explain, at least has some phenotypic expression like you're talking about that startle response that would help people sort.

 

305

00:46:29.630 --> 00:46:46.620

Kris Hiney: This horse needs to be in the hands of somebody who is much more attuned and sympathetic to that kind of reactivity, and can excel like you said so. The humans that excelled versus I could see that same horse in the wrong environment would be in real jeopardy. Right.

 

306

00:46:46.620 --> 00:47:08.919

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, yeah, yeah, absolutely. So that's that's part of what we're doing now is that in the interim before we have a nice genetic test to to give a prediction that would be. You know, you send off some hairs and you get back a percentage of always 70 likely to be a challenge in this situation. Right? In the meantime, if we improve our measures of phenotype, we can begin to select on phenotype specifically so.

 

307

00:47:09.440 --> 00:47:25.999

Samantha Brooks: Dr. Wickens had a lovely student, Ellen Rankins, who did a bit of work. Yeah, they they did. And now Ellen's up at Rutgers, right. So part of what she did when she was here at Florida was look at ways to measure behavioral reactivity in therapy horses.

 

308

00:47:26.860 --> 00:47:43.129

Samantha Brooks: So that, you know, when you got a new therapy horse in, you could do things like like measure their skin sensitivity to calibrated touch and measure their how they react to a uniform behavioral trial like show them a novel object

 

309

00:47:43.210 --> 00:48:11.969

Samantha Brooks: to see if there are ways that that we could start to measure this consistently animal to animal and over time, so that we can select on phenotypes with some sort of an organized system until we get better knowledge of of the genetics. And and that's where we're at with the locomotion project, too, is that while long term, I really want to make these tools, so I can do genetics in the short term. It gives us a better way to measure the phenotype, and then you can select on that phenotype.

 

310

00:48:12.010 --> 00:48:18.350

Samantha Brooks: both as breeders and as potential purchasers and as managers. Right? Because phenotype can change day to day.

 

311

00:48:18.850 --> 00:48:29.600

Samantha Brooks: you can look at your interaction between a genetically influenced phenotype and the environment. If you have a uniform way to measure it day to day to day, just like we have a weght tape

 

312

00:48:29.870 --> 00:48:40.169

Samantha Brooks: for for body, condition and and body weight estimates. It'd be nice to have something as simple and repeatable and inexpensive for some of these

 

313

00:48:40.950 --> 00:48:43.080

Samantha Brooks: more complex traits.

 

314

00:48:43.080 --> 00:49:03.279

Kris Hiney: Yeah, I just think that, like the future that I would envision is, you're like he has been tested to be 70% 4-H horse. But like, I just think there's such a population need for that kind of animal, because, like the performance ones, need to be twitchier. But we need this population of

 

315

00:49:03.760 --> 00:49:05.909

Kris Hiney: non reactors, so to speak, which.

 

316

00:49:05.910 --> 00:49:06.400

Samantha Brooks: Yeah.

 

317

00:49:06.805 --> 00:49:07.210

Kris Hiney: Wish.

 

318

00:49:07.440 --> 00:49:35.329

Samantha Brooks: Absolutely like people say, Oh, I never! You know. Why would we? Wanna why would we wanna breathe the reactivity out of our horses, thinking of like a genetic disease, and I'm like, no, no, no, no, no, no, no! The fun thing about horses and roll. One reason why we still have so much genetic diversity within horses that we've lost somewhat in some of our dog breeds is that they all do very, very different jobs. And it's not about building the perfect horse. It's about finding the right horse to the right job.

 

319

00:49:35.610 --> 00:50:03.970

Samantha Brooks: So for some animals, it's gonna be more reactivity. And for other animals. Yeah, it's it's gonna be, it's gonna be less reactivity. And and anytime you can get a little bit of extra guidance about what job is gonna be best suited to. It's gonna save a lot of time, right? Because I feel like these days, we don't have as many horse people. And I think that the research supports this. We don't have as many horse people who grew up around horses, and and it can take years, if not decades.

 

320

00:50:03.970 --> 00:50:11.100

Samantha Brooks: to calibrate your human brain and eyes to be able to assess intuitively

 

321

00:50:11.150 --> 00:50:18.539

Samantha Brooks: reactivity reactivity in a horse in a fairly short amount of time. We just don't have as many humans that have that kind of

 

322

00:50:19.290 --> 00:50:28.269

Samantha Brooks: carefully practiced skill anymore. So it's probably time we started to use some technologies to help all the rest of us

 

323

00:50:28.460 --> 00:50:30.339

Samantha Brooks: be able to to work that magic.

 

324

00:50:30.602 --> 00:50:40.307

Kris Hiney: Because I think you know, selection of a horse is so often an emotional response. But, man, if you had data like, if you're like. You can't look at this horse until you look at his data.

 

325

00:50:40.570 --> 00:50:46.840

Samantha Brooks: Yes, it is such such an emotional thing. It really, really, really is. So

 

326

00:50:46.850 --> 00:50:55.029

Samantha Brooks: anytime. And I tell people I'm like, don't go and buy a car without a car fax, and I don't even fax them anymore. But we still call them the Carfax. Right?

 

327

00:50:55.430 --> 00:51:07.150

Samantha Brooks: Try not to buy a horse without something like a comprehensive genetic report. It doesn't necessarily mean that it's going to completely sway your your opinion one way or another, but

 

328

00:51:07.220 --> 00:51:20.370

Samantha Brooks: it's definitely gonna help to guide you through the process. Maybe give you maybe give you a better, a better sense of confidence in your decision. Or maybe it's gonna feed that little niggling, tiny

 

329

00:51:20.540 --> 00:51:26.479

Samantha Brooks: kernel of worry in the back of your mind like, Hey, I'm not sure this one is a right fit. Should I take the risk.

 

330

00:51:26.970 --> 00:51:27.350

Kris Hiney: Yeah.

 

331

00:51:27.350 --> 00:51:36.869

Samantha Brooks: Maybe that generic reports gonna help just kind of, you know, sway you into listening to that worry and maybe avoid a match. That's that's not. That's not perfect.

 

332

00:51:36.870 --> 00:51:48.669

Kris Hiney: I'm hoping you can get us to like, you know, where there's horses that can be obese and horses that can't right. That's that's genetic. We need some markers. Stat.

 

333

00:51:49.050 --> 00:52:01.809

Samantha Brooks: Oh, I know, boy, we try. We did. We? We worked on that for quite a while, because it is such a pervasive problem, and I had a strong interest in metabolic syndrome, and I would keep working on it for sure if I could just get it funded. But

 

334

00:52:01.830 --> 00:52:02.999

Samantha Brooks: sort of the

 

335

00:52:03.310 --> 00:52:18.599

Samantha Brooks: the the attention span of our of our funders is pretty short, so laminitis and metabolic syndrome seems to have waned somewhat in the fundable topics. Category. But yeah, same same thing, you know, and even if you have one that tends to be fat, well.

 

336

00:52:18.970 --> 00:52:26.460

Samantha Brooks: send him send him somewhere where you have fairly poor quality pasture, and you're really concerned about your budget

 

337

00:52:26.630 --> 00:52:33.800

Samantha Brooks: and get him in the right environment, and he'll be in a great situation, and the one that tends to be thinner. Well, send them to Kentucky in the spring.

 

338

00:52:33.800 --> 00:52:36.550

Kris Hiney: Exactly. Exactly. Yeah, yeah.

 

339

00:52:36.764 --> 00:52:37.620

Samantha Brooks: Be in good shape.

 

340

00:52:37.620 --> 00:53:02.570

Kris Hiney: Well, I really appreciate this was a fun conversation like it it actually, it's got my brain turning about some other things I might need to add to some of my courses on, like, I just am fascinated with like behavior. And like, Hmm! There's really some things you have to pay attention to, and not just a one. Size fits all like I think, that super important to me, and to have a geneticist be like.

 

341

00:53:02.570 --> 00:53:08.134

Kris Hiney: Oh, cool. It's back here in some DNA.

 

342

00:53:08.830 --> 00:53:18.299

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, yeah, for sure. It's there's there's definitely something. There's definitely something to it. And I am hoping that we'll get caught up on the

 

343

00:53:18.450 --> 00:53:46.450

Samantha Brooks: on the horse Research, where we are with with lab rats and our model species. But it's funny. I'll tell you. It's really is one of my collaborators works in. She's a human researcher works on humans. And you know, she's just as frustrated because it seems like the human medical community is coming late to this realization as well. So we're not so far behind the 8 ball. But it'll, it'd be really good to to get a grasp, but.

 

344

00:53:46.740 --> 00:53:49.746

Kris Hiney: Have you guys tried the lotto as a maybe funding agency.

 

345

00:53:50.020 --> 00:53:51.440

Samantha Brooks: You know it's funny.

 

346

00:53:52.650 --> 00:54:08.029

Samantha Brooks: I I I my tickets, not here. It's in a kitchen. But my graduate students. This is this is a running joke is that you know, we buy lottery ticket tickets strictly so that Dr. Brooks can say, when I win the lottery, this is the research program we're gonna run.

 

347

00:54:08.580 --> 00:54:15.396

Kris Hiney: Well, I tell you what, if you're like the amount of time I spend on writing a grant or buying tickets, which one is gonna pay up for.

 

348

00:54:15.850 --> 00:54:24.091

Samantha Brooks: Right in that cost, you know. Cost time. Ratio cost benefit ratio there. Yeah. The lottery ticket sometimes looks like the better payoff.

 

349

00:54:24.750 --> 00:54:25.050

Kris Hiney: Yeah.

 

350

00:54:25.050 --> 00:54:32.489

Samantha Brooks: So my behavior stuff I've I've sent it to Nsf. Several times. I'm going to rewrite it this summer for my fourth submission to Nsf.

 

351

00:54:32.590 --> 00:54:46.000

Samantha Brooks: And last summer I was at a meeting and I talked to an Nsf. Program coordinator, and I said, Hey, I'm kind of frustrated. I've sent in 3 times. I feel like I'm getting nowhere. And he goes. Oh, like 3 times I said, Yeah, 3 times, he said, well, you know, we just ran the statistics, and he said, when it comes to Nsf.

 

352

00:54:46.070 --> 00:54:50.590

Samantha Brooks: The Median number of submissions to an award is 7.

 

353

00:54:50.590 --> 00:54:52.270

Kris Hiney: Holy buckets!

 

354

00:54:52.520 --> 00:55:02.289

Samantha Brooks: 7 years. And and so then I was like, Okay, I don't feel so bad. And then he's like and get this, he said the range. It went from one to 17 years.

 

355

00:55:02.290 --> 00:55:03.020

Kris Hiney: Wow!

 

356

00:55:03.390 --> 00:55:16.869

Samantha Brooks: Somebody submitted a project 17 years in a row, or 17 submissions in a row, I guess. Sometimes they come around more than once, but sometimes they come around only every 2 years. If we have a Federal Government shutdown right? Somebody submitted the same project 17 times before it got funded. So I'm like.

 

357

00:55:17.540 --> 00:55:20.589

Kris Hiney: I bet you they didn't have a heavy teaching load.

 

358

00:55:20.590 --> 00:55:21.570

Samantha Brooks: You know. Right?

 

359

00:55:23.010 --> 00:55:34.360

Samantha Brooks: Right? Yes. How well they have the time to revise at 17 years in a row. Yeah, this is well, this is why this gets delegated to summer right summer. I'm a 9 month employee. Right? So summer is my.

 

360

00:55:34.360 --> 00:55:34.930

Kris Hiney: You're freaking.

 

361

00:55:34.930 --> 00:55:38.880

Samantha Brooks: Not paid to do anything, so I'll volunteer my time writing grants.

 

362

00:55:41.260 --> 00:55:43.489

Kris Hiney: Oh, it's fun out there, but.

 

363

00:55:43.490 --> 00:55:50.740

Samantha Brooks: Yeah, yeah, it's it's it's just a blast, just a blast. But yes, we joke all the time at it.

 

364

00:55:51.210 --> 00:55:52.910

Samantha Brooks: When I win the lottery.

 

365

00:55:53.420 --> 00:55:54.969

Samantha Brooks: Then we'll get to have a

 

366

00:55:55.150 --> 00:55:57.940

Samantha Brooks: the research program that we've all dreamed of.

 

367

00:55:57.940 --> 00:56:17.150

Kris Hiney: Yeah. Well, again, Dr. Brooks really appreciate your time. I know you're super busy, as like, I said, leading equine geneticist. Everybody knows we really appreciate the work that you're doing for the worse industry, cause it. It's it's tough and applaud any information that you're able to send our way.

 

368

00:56:17.160 --> 00:56:20.469

Samantha Brooks: Absolutely. It's been a lot of fun, and just let me know if you want to do it again.

 

369

00:56:20.610 --> 00:56:21.920

Kris Hiney: Yeah, absolutely.